"Truthiness" is a term created by comedian
Stephen Colbert and Merriam-Webster’s 2006 word of the
year: “the quality of seeming to be true according to one’s
intuition…without regard to logic [or] factual evidence.” Now science is figuring out how to make statements seeming truer than they are, such as including images or charts - so it's only a matter of time before this filters into marketing and political ads (I suspect marketers are way ahead of the psychologists on this one).
Researchers find a simple way to make statements seem truer
Trust but verify Image: iStock/zmina
After each of the presidential debates, the media has scrambled to decide, among other things, which of the two candidates was more truthful. The fact-checkers worked hard, attempting to establish if anything President Obama or Governor Romney said was inaccurate. However, it isn’t as if the rest of us waited for the morning paper to make our own judgments. Whether watching the debates, viewing the commercials, or reading campaign literature, we always have a sense for how truthful a politician candidate is. Where does this sense come from?
Late-night television satirist Stephen Colbert
urges his audience to rely on their gut for what he has dubbed a
feeling of “truthiness.” Truthiness, Merriam-Webster’s 2006 word of the
year, is “the quality of seeming to be true according to one’s
intuition…without regard to logic [or] factual evidence.” Although
Colbert deserves credit for coining the word, psychologists have long
known that people rely on their feelings to draw all sorts of
conclusions, and a recent paper
clarifies one situation that seems to lead us to strong feelings of
truthiness – the presence of additional related (but irrelevant)
information.
The research finds that a statement in the presence of images or other
additional information enhances people’s feelings of truthiness, even
when they don’t provide any evidence the statement is true. This is
especially important in the context of political campaigns, as it
suggests that that the mere presence of a picture next to a candidate’s
written claims could lead people to be more likely to believe them. And
the work is another demonstration of the ease with which our thoughts,
beliefs, and behaviors can be manipulated through relatively innocuous
means.
The authors, researchers from Victoria University of Wellington, Kwantlen Polytechnic University, and University of Victoria,
performed four experiments. In the first three studies, participants
viewed names of celebrities, displayed one at a time. Some of the names
also had a picture or a short verbal description attached. Finally, half
of the participants judged the truth of the statement “this famous
person is alive,” while the rest judged the truth of, “this famous
person is dead.” The participants were more likely to judge a statement
as true when it was accompanied by a picture or by a short description,
regardless of whether the statement was that the individual was alive,
or that the individual was dead. The effect was stronger for less
familiar celebrities.
In a related experiment, the researchers showed the effect was not
particular to celebrities. Participants viewed trivia statements, some
of which were accompanied by related photos which provided no evidence
of the truth of the statement, and indicated whether they thought the
statement was true or false. For example, next to the statement
“macadamia nuts are in the same evolutionary family as peaches,” a
participant might see a picture of macadamia nuts. The photos increased
the bias toward rating statements as true.
The fact that irrelevant pictures alter our perceptions of truth is
related to a general principle about the way our minds work. Our
judgments are based on not only the information we’re considering, but
the way in which that information is processed and organized. The ease
with which information is processed has long been known
to lead to specific biases. The reasoning works as follows: when
considering some piece of new information, an individual will attempt to
remember other bits of consistent information. The more easily these
bits of information are retrieved, the more likely the new information
is going to be tagged as true. So, if you are told, “an ostrich’s eye is
bigger than it’s brain,” you will attempt to recall all the information
you know about ostriches, eyes, and brains. The easier you bring this
information to mind, the more likely you are to decide that the
statement is true (spoiler: it’s true).
This ease-of-recall is known as fluency, and the effect of fluency is
extremely wide-ranging. While the present paper shows that we judge
fluent information as more true, previous work has shown that fluently
processed faces are judged to be more attractive, and fluently processed names more frequent. In fact, the Nobel prize was awarded to Daniel Kahneman 2002 for work which showed, among other things, that the ease with which we can bring information to mind leads to an assortment of biases in decision making.
Though the authors believe that this fluency explanation is the most
likely one, it is impossible at this stage to rule out other
explanations. The authors speculate that it is possible, for example,
that the pictures or text could lead people to preferentially look for
evidence consistent with the statement. For example, if presented with a
photo of a celebrity, and the statement, “This famous person is alive,”
one might seek out elements of the picture which provide support for
the statement that he or she is alive, and ignore those elements of the
picture which might suggest that he or she is not alive, like signs the
picture comes from a previous decade. Further work should be able to
disentangle which explanation is more correct.
With profound apologies to Colbert, these findings suggest we would all
be wise to be more critical of our feelings of truthiness. Is that
health claim on your cereal box accompanied by a picture? Do the safety
claims of the car ad in your magazine appear alongside other information
about the vehicle? Does the assertion of a fact on a website appear
next to a photo of the writer? Given that we will live with the
consequences of this presidential elections for the next four years, we
should pay close attention not only to the information presented by the
candidates, but also the manner in which they present that information.
There are many instances in which trusting the truth which comes from
your gut could mean that you’re subscribing to something less than the
truth. In other words: if it feels good, question it.
Are you a scientist who specializes in neuroscience, cognitive
science, or psychology? And have you read a recent peer-reviewed paper
that you would like to write about? Please send suggestions to Mind
Matters editor Gareth Cook, a Pulitzer prize-winning journalist at the
Boston Globe. He can be reached at garethideas AT gmail.com or Twitter @garethideas.
ABOUT THE AUTHOR(S)
Travis Riddle is a doctoral student in the psychology department at Columbia University. His work in the Sparrow Lab
focuses on the sense of control people have over their thoughts and
actions, and the perceptual and self-regulatory consequences of this
sense of control.
No comments:
Post a Comment